TODAY'S CONSIDERATIONS
So what other different perspectives exist among the masses in general which are not held by "the realized"?
For years, the pointer has been offered that (1) there is no one here who is special or Special and that (2) contrary to widespread opinion, there is no one anywhere who is special or Special. "Contrary" applies because most are convinced that they are special, superior, and moral.
Again, to the contrary,
Maharaj said: "Only that person will visit this place whose virtue and sin have come to an end." See? No duality. Not moral. Not immoral. There is no universal standard by which either of those could be defined with total agreement and without any debate anyway.
In an article entitled "Most People Think They’re Morally Superior To Everyone Else," a key point offered is that most think they are "so high and mighty" that they "can feel entitled to commit immoral acts."
The article continued: "In the privacy of your own mind, do you believe you’re a better person than everyone else? Well, that’s pretty much what the rest of us think, too."
A recent study published in the journal Social Psychological and Personality Science shows that people strongly believe that they are just, virtuous and moral, and uniformly see others as inferior. People also tend to rate themselves more highly than others in modesty.
The study, first published in October and resurfaced this week in Scientific American, showed that most people fail to use reason when evaluating character traits in themselves and others.
“The individuals in our sample consistently judged themselves to be superior to the average person,” Ben Tappin, a psychologist at the University of London and the study’s lead author, told The Huffington Post.
For the study, participants were asked to rate the extent to which 30 different personality traits were present in themselves, the average person, and a “socially desirable” ideal. This included traits involved in morality (like sincerity and honesty), sociability (like friendliness), and personal agency (like competence and creativity).
Then, the researchers conducted a test to determine how rational the participants were in their reasoning.
Here’s what rational thinking on the matter would look like:
Most of the time, most people are in the majority. In order to make accurate judgments about others, it would be reasonable to project what we know about ourselves. Therefore, we’re rational when we acknowledge how similar we are to other people.
But there’s a caveat in this for people who are truly unusual. If a person rates themselves in an atypical way, then they are objectively unusual. So, in that case, it would make sense for them to rate themselves differently from others.
“Those individuals who were highly atypical had more ‘room’ for rational self-enhancement because their own traits provided a poor guide as to what the average person was like,” Tappin said.
People were most likely to be irrational when it came to traits that they considered desirable ― especially moral traits, the researchers found.
This can be explained in part by what psychologists call the “self-enhancement effect.”
“Self-enhancement has been studied for decades, and refers to the phenomenon whereby people perceive themselves to be superior to the average person,” Tappin said. “The classic case was documented . . . back in 1981 in the case of driving ability. As we discuss in the paper, the effect has since been observed in many other domains including personality traits, and intelligence.”
One possible explanation for how these positive illusions came to be is the idea that they confer psychological benefits, like increased confidence and self-esteem, Tappin said.
But the researchers found no evidence of this. Instead, they observed that feelings of moral superiority were not associated with high self-esteem.
Falsely imagining ourselves to be morally superior also can result in “moral licensing” ― the tendency for people to feel justified in doing something immoral because they generally feel they behave morally. One study, for instance, showed that people who committed a moral act were more likely to commit an immoral act later the4 same day(perhaps because they felt entitled to do so).
It’s a good reminder for all of us that thinking you’re a “moral person” and actually behaving morally aren’t the same thing. Moreover, the realized do not believe that either they or anyone else is moral and do not believe that they or others are immoral.
Again, "Only that person will visit this place whose virtue and sin have come to an end." Anything belief contrary to that understanding is based in the claptrap and gobbledygook of dualistic thinking.
The different perspective of those who have received the proper treatment for the Ultimate Sickness results in their having lost all beliefs, including the belief in personhood, specialness and Specialness, too.
To be continued.
Please enter into the silence of contemplation.
[NOTE: The four most recent posts are below. You may access all of the posts in this series and in the previous series and several thousand other posts as well by clicking on the links in the "Recent Posts and Archives" section.]
In addition to the five non-duality books made available without charge by Andy Gugar, Jr. (see “FREEBIES” above), you can now access nearly 2,900 posts for any topics of interest to you.